2.4 Changes in behaviour help explain changes in HIV prevalence
As discussed earlier, changing behaviour and a consequent
reduction in new HIV infections is just one possible reason for changes in HIV
prevalence data. It is, of course, the most encouraging explanation to those
trying to reduce the spread of the virus. But without collecting data that show
trends in behaviour over time, program evaluators will not be able to ascertain
whether behaviour change contributes to changes in HIV prevalence.
Focusing entirely on HIV prevalence without complementary
behavioural data can also be misleading. When HIV prevalence stabilises - and
even when it stabilises at very high levels - there is often a tendency to
become complacent: the problem has peaked, it won't get any worse. This can be a
dangerous fallacy. For example, prevalence among injecting drug users in Bangkok
has been stable for almost a decade, but careful studies of cohorts of drug
users have shown that they continue to become infected at a rate between 5 and
10 percent per year. Stable prevalence results because the number of newly
infected drug users roughly equals the number dropping out due to death and to
ceasing injection.
Behavioural data showing no change in high levels of risk
activities, or continued risk in certain age groups or sections of the
population, should ring alarm bells even when HIV prevalence seems stable.
Several factors unrelated to intervention effects can contribute to observed
stabilisation or decreases in HIV prevalence in a given setting. These include
mortality (especially in mature epidemics), saturation effects in subpopulations
at higher risk, differential migration patterns related to the epidemic,
sampling bias, and errors in data collection and
analysis.